بیجنخان، محمود (1397). فرهنگ بسامدی: براساس پیکره متنی زبان فارسی امروز. تهران: انتشارات دانشگاه تهران.
Anderson, J. R. (1983). The Architecture of Cognition. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.
Barber, H. & M. Carreiras (2005). "Grammatical gender and number agreement in spanish: An ERP comparison". Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience 17(1), 137–153.
Batterink, L, C. M. Karns, Y. Yamada, & Neville (2012). "The Role of Awareness in Semantic and Syntactic Processing: An ERP Attentional Blink Study". Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 29, 2514- 2529.
Berkovitch, L. & S. Dehaene (2019). "Subliminal syntactic priming". Cognitive Psychology. 109, 26–46. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cogpsych.2018.12.001
Blackwood, D. H. & W. J. Muir (1990). "Cognitive brain potentials and their application". The British Journal of Psychiatry, 157(Suppl 9), 96-101.
Canseco-Gonzalez, E. (2000). "Using the recording of event-related brain potentials in the study of sentence processing". Language and the brain: Representation and processing. Y. Grodzinsky, L. P. Shapiro, & D. Swinney (eds.), New York: Academic Press, 229-260.
DeKeyser, R. M. (2001). "Automaticity and automatization". Cognition and Second Language Instruction. P. Robinson (Ed.), New York: Cambridge University Press, 125–151.
Dehaene, S. et al. (2003). "three parietal circuits for number processing". Cognitive Neuropsychology. 20(3-6), 487-506.
Folstein, J. R. & C. van Petten (2008). "Influence of cognitive control and mismatch on the N2 component of the ERP: A review". Psychophysiology. 45, 152–170.
Friederici, A. D. (1995). "The time course of syntactic activation during language processing: A model based on neuropsychological and neurophysiological data". Brain and Language. 50, 259–281
Friederici, A. D., B. Opitz, & Y. D. von Cramon (2002). "Segregating semantic and syntactic aspects of processing in the human brain: An fMRI investigation of different word types". Cerebral Cortex. 10(7), 698-705.
Friederici, A. D., E. Pfeifer, & A. Hahne (1993). "Event-related brain potentials during natural speech processing: Effects of semantic, morphological and syntactic violations". Cognitive Brain Research, 1, 183–192.
Goodman, G. O., J. L. McClelland, & R. W. Gibbs (1981). "The role of syntactic content in word recognition". Memory & Cognition. 9, 580–586.
Hahne, A. & A. D. Friederici (1999). "Electrophysiological evidence for two steps in syntactic analysis: Early automatic and late controlled processes". Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience. 11, 194–205.
Hartsuiker, R. J. & A. Moors (2017). "On the automaticity of language processing". Entrenchment and the Psychology of Language Learning: How We Reorganize and Adapt Linguistic Knowledge. H. J. Schmid (ed.), American Psychological Association; De Gruyter Mouton, 201–225.
Iijima, K. & K. L. Sakai (2014). "Subliminal enhancement of predictive effects during syntactic processing in the left inferior frontal gyrus: An MEG study". Frontiers in Systems Neuroscience. 8, 217.
Kahneman, D. (1973). Attention and Effort. Englewood Cliffs, NJ: Prentice-Hall.
Kiefer, M., N. M. Trumpp, C. Schaitz, H. Reuss, & W. Kunde (2019). "Attentional modulation of masked semantic priming by visible and masked task cues". Cognition. 187, 62–77.
Lehtonen, M., P. J. Monahan, & D. Poeppel (2011). "Evidence for Early Morphological Decomposition: Combining Masked Priming with Magnetoencephalography". Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 23(11), 3366–3379.
Levelt, W. J. M., A. Roelofs, & A. S. Meyer (1998). "A theory of lexical access in speech production". Behavioral and Brain Sciences. 22, 1–75.
Logan, G. D. (1988). "Toward an instance theory of automatization". Psychological Review, 95, 492–527. doi:10. 1037/0033-295X.95.4.492.
Marslen-Wilson, W. D., M. Bozic, & B. Randall (2008). "Early decomposition in visual word recognition: Dissociating morphology, form, and meaning". Language, Cognition and Neuroscience, 23(3), 394–421.
Münte, T. J., H. Heinze, & G. Mangun (1993). "Dissociation of brain activity related to syntactic and semantic aspects of language". Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 5, 335–344.
Paradis, M. (2009). Declarative and Procedural Determinants of Second Languages (Vol. 40). Amsterdam: John Benjamins.
Schmidt, R. (1994). "Deconstructing consciousness in search of useful definitions for applied linguistics". Consciousness and Second Language Learning: Conceptual, Methodological and Practical Issues in Language Learning and Teaching, Thematic Issue of AILA Review—Revue de l’AILA. J. H. Hulstijn & R. Schmidt (eds.), 11, 11–26.
Segalowitz, N. (2003). "Automaticity and second language learning". The Handbook of Second Language Acquisition. C. Doughty & M. Long (eds.), Oxford: Blackwell. 10.1002/9780470756492, 382–408
Segalowitz, N. (2010). The Cognitive Bases of Second Language Fluency. New York: Routledge.
Seidenberg, M. S. (1997). "Language acquisition and use: Learning and applying probabilistic constraints". Science. 275 (5306), 1599–1603.
Seidenberg, M. S., et al. (1984). "Pre- and post-lexical loci of contextual effects on word recognition". Memory & Cognition. 12, 315–328.
Sereno, J. A. (1991). "Graphemic, associative, and syntactic priming effects at a brief stimulus onset asynchrony in lexical decision and naming". Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition. 17, 459–477.
Van den Bussche, E., W. Van den Noortgate, & B. Reynvoet (2009). "Mechanisms of masked priming: A meta-analysis". Psychological Bulleti., 135(3), 452–477.
Vogel, E. K., S. J. Luck, & K. L. Shapiro (1998). "Electrophysiological evidence for postperceptual locus of suppression during the attentional blink". Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance. 24(6), 1656- 1674.
Wright, B. & M. Garrett (1984). "Lexical decision in sentences: Effects of syntactic structure". Journal of Memory and Cognition. 12, 31–45.
Yamada, Y. & H. Neville (2007). "An ERP study of syntactic processing in English and nonsense sentences". Brain Research. 1130, 167-180.